Hassan, A., Sohn, G., & MacKenzie, I. S. (2023). Comparison of one-handed and two-handed text entry in virtual reality using handheld controllers. Proceedings of the 14th International Conference on Applied Human Factors and Ergonomics -- AHFE '23, New York: AHFE International. pp. 101-111. doi:10.54941/ahfe1003872 [PDF]
Comparison of One-handed and Two-handed Text Entry in Virtual Reality Using Handheld Controllers
Ali Hassan1, Gunho Sohn1, I. Scott MacKenzie2
1Dept. of Earth and Space Science, York University, Toronto, Canada
2Dept. of Electrical Engineering and Computer Science, York University, Toronto, Canada
Figure 1: VR text entry techniques compared: standard (1H) and split (2H) keyboards.ABSTRACT
The efficiency of text input on virtual reality (VR) systems was evaluated using handheld controllers with two virtual keyboards: a split two-handed keyboard and a standard one-handed keyboard. The results of a user study with 14 participants showed that the split two-handed keyboard was significantly faster with an average speed of 15.2 words per minute, which was 7.5% faster than the standard one-handed keyboard. Both keyboards had similar error rates and keystrokes per character. Preference ratings were evenly split between the two keyboards. On favourability, the standard one-handed keyboard received an average approval rating of 8.1 out of 10, compared to 7.2 out of 10 for the split two-handed keyboard.KEYWORDS
human-computer interaction, text input, virtual reality, user experience, controllers
INTRODUCTION
Advancements in virtual reality (VR) hardware have led to an increase in the adoption and demand for VR headsets. Currently, consumer VR systems rely heavily on handheld controllers for input, including text input, which can be a tedious and error-prone process that requires users to coordinate multiple inputs (McGill, 2015). In response, researchers have investigated alternative methods for improving text input in VR, including a physical and virtual keyboard, predictive typing, and gesture-based input (Bowman, 2002; Dube, 2019; Boletsis, 2019). These techniques have been developed and studied since the emergence of VR to enhance the user experience for activities such as gaming or text entry while wearing a head-mounted display (HMD) and using handheld controllers.Researchers have also employed traditional physical Qwerty keyboards for use in VR (Walker, 2017; Grubet, 2018), but this approach is not practical due to the need for wireless VR headsets that allow users to move freely without being tethered to a desk. In addition, using a physical keyboard while wearing an HMD can be challenging as users are unable to see the keys. Although speech-to-text input has improved with advances in speech recognition, this approach raises concerns about privacy as users may not feel comfortable speaking aloud sensitive information. Other studies have proposed using dwell-based gestures for input, but these gestures require significant body movement and may cause fatigue (Yu, 2017).
Handheld controllers in VR typically use raycasting, where the controller casts a virtual ray with targets selected using an aim-and-shoot principle. See Figure 1. For text entry, techniques include soft keyboards, drum-like keyboards, gesture- or dwell-based input, and split keyboards. However, there is little experimental research comparing keyboard interfaces for VR. The present study aims to address this gap by presenting an empirical evaluation of split two-handed and standard one-handed virtual keyboards for text input in VR systems using handheld controllers. Both the split two-handed and standard one-handed virtual keyboards were specifically designed and evaluated for VR text input, with potential applications such as enabling users to enter their gamer names or messaging with other gamers.
Split Keyboards
The split keyboard (see Figure 2), originating in the 1960s, offers an ergonomic alternative to a standard Qwerty setup and reduces the risk of repetitive strain injuries (Rempel, 2008). Additionally, split keyboards reduce muscle strain on the shoulders and neck through physical adjustments of the keyboard assembly (Smith, 2015). They are commonly used in office settings.
Figure 2: Qwerty split keyboard layout.
Related Work
With reference to Table 1, there are various methods for inputting text in VR, each with pros and cons. Many consumer VR systems, such as the HTC Vive, Oculus Rift, Samsung Gear, and Playstation VR, come equipped with interactive features like hand-held controllers, eye and head tracking, which have been used in various studies to explore different VR text input methods. Walker et al. (2017) looked at the use of a completely occluded keyboard for typing in VR. The study involved 24 participants, and the average text entry rate ranged from 41.2 to 43.7 words per minute (wpm) with a mean character error rate of 8.4% to 11.8%. These error rates were reduced to approximately 2.6% to 4.0% when the typed input was auto-corrected using a decoder. Typing in VR can be difficult for users because they cannot see their hands while wearing a head-mounted display (HMD), which can affect speed and accuracy (Grubert et al., 2018).
Table 1. Text entry research and methods in VR.
Study Short Description np1 ns2 Entry Speed
(wpm)Error rate
(%)Walker et al.
(2017)Occluded keyboard in VR: with auto-
correct, without auto-correct24 2 41.2, 43.7 11.8, 8.4 Grubert et al.
(2018)Physical keyboard, virtual keyboard 24 1 26.3, 11.6 2.1, 2.7 McGill et al.
(2015)Typing in enhanced virtuality: partial
physical, full physical keyboard16 1 36.6, 38.5 10.4, 9.2 Speicher et al.
(2018)Handheld controller techniques in VR:
head pointing, controller pointing,
controller tapping, freehand, discrete
cursor, continuous cursor24 1 10.2, 15.4,
12.7, 9.8,
5.3, 8.41.2, 0.97,
1.9, 7.6,
2.8, 2.2He et al.
(2022)Gaze gestures with disambiguation on
VR: Qwerty, TapGazer without and
with word completion12 5 52.6, 42.3,
42.811.5, 2.1,
3.4Rajanna & Hansen
(2018)Eye gaze for text input in VR: dwell,
click16 4 9.4, 10.2 0.02, 0.07 Chen et al.
(2019)Gesture-based input for text input in
VR: controllers, touchscreen10 1 16.4, 9.6 0.12, 0.24 1np = number of participants; 2ns = number of sessions Grubert et al. (2018) conducted a study to investigate the effect of various hand representations on text input in four different scenarios: no representation, animated hands, fingers, and video inlay of the hands. They found that accuracy with video inlay and fingers was significantly higher, but the representation of the hands did not significantly affect input speed. McGill et al. (2015) compared typing on a desktop keyboard in enhanced virtuality using a complete keyboard view in VR, no keyboard view, partial view, and full blending conditions in reality.
Text input in VR can also utilize head pointing, which is a built-in interaction featured in many consumer VR headsets. The main distinction is that with the head pointing the cursor is controlled with head movement rather than hand movement using controllers. To input a character, users first move the cursor to the desired key and then select the appropriate character by either dwelling on it for a predetermined time or using a trigger press (Speicher et al., 2018). Many modern VR headsets also have eye trackers, allowing for the use of eye movement as a text input method. Rajanna and Hansen (2018) examined eye gazing as a text input method in VR and found that users performed better when the complete keyboard was in view and when they were stationary, rather than moving. They also found that the gaze+click interaction was superior to dwell-only interaction.
He et al. (2022) proposed a technique that used gaze gestures to disambiguate text input as users typed by tapping their fingers in a specific location. This allowed users to tap anywhere as long as their finger identity was easily recognizable. This prototype achieved an entry speed of 79.2% of users' Qwerty typing speed, but it had limitations such as word prediction based only on word frequency and no auto-correction.
Another technique for improving text input in virtual reality (VR) is gesture-based input, which allows users to input text by making hand gestures. However, this method can be difficult for users to learn and may not be suitable for all types of text input tasks (Chen et al., 2019).
Currently, handheld controllers are a common method for text input in consumer VR systems. Speicher et al. (2018) studied various techniques for using handheld controllers for input, including controller pointing, controller tapping, discrete or continuous cursor movement, and trigger-press selection.
The present study focuses on handheld controllers as a text input method, comparing the efficiency and effectiveness of controller pointing and controller selection techniques. The goal was to determine which technique is better for text input in VR.
In summary, text input in VR using handheld controllers remains a challenging task, and more research is needed to identify the most effective and efficient methods for inputting text in virtual environments.
METHOD
An empirical evaluation of two virtual keyboards using VR handheld controllers was conducted to compare their text-entry speed, accuracy, and efficiency. Both keyboards were Qwerty-based with different layouts. The standard one-handed keyboard had all the keys in a single location, like most physical Qwerty keyboards. On the other hand, the split two-handed keyboard had keys on both sides of the screen, divided into two separate sections.
Participants
Students from a local university were voluntarily recruited to participate in the study. A total of 14 participants were recruited, including 7 males and 7 females. The mean age was 28.0 years (SD = 5.0). The volunteers had to be present in person to participate in the study. Participation was entirely voluntary, and no financial compensation was given.
Apparatus
This study used Meta Quest 2 as a VR headset, along with its stock hand-held controllers. Meta Quest 2 is a lightweight (285 g) consumer VR headset that does not require a computer to be connected to it. The interfaces were developed using the Unity3D game engine with the input cursor disabled. Text phrases were selected randomly from the MacKenzie and Soukoreff (2003) phrase set. C# scripts tracked keystrokes and timings while users entered the phrase. These values were recorded in a text file and later used to determine the entry speed, accuracy, and efficiency of both text-entry methods.Both VR text-input techniques had similar typing environments. A text entry box with a phrase appeared in the center of the screen. The standard one-handed keyboard was placed directly beneath the text box. In the case of split two-handed keyboards, the right and left portions of the keyboard were placed on the right and left sides of the text-entry area, as shown in Figure 3.
(a) (b)
Figure 3: Text input using ray-casting with virtual keyboards. (a) Standard keyboard using a single controller, (b) Split keyboard utilizing both left and right controllers.
Procedure
Participants were directed to take a seat at a desk equipped with a computer and a Meta Quest 2 VR headset, as shown in Figure 4.
Figure 4: Setup. (1) User wearing Meta Quest 2 HMD, (2) Meta touch controllers, (3) Optional PC connected to Meta HMD, (4) User clicks trigger to select a letter from virtual keyboard.They were informed about the goals and objectives of the study and cautioned about the potential risks associated with VR, including motion sickness and dizziness. To participate, participants were required to sign informed consent forms. A demographic questionnaire was administered to gather information about the participants' experience with VR, allowing for the differentiation between VR novices and more experienced individuals.
Prior to being presented with the first text input method, participants were given time to practice the text input process. They were then divided into two groups to mitigate any potential learning effects.
The formal experiment included five blocks of trials for each text input method, each block comprising five phrases from the MacKenzie and Soukoreff (2003) phrase set. During each trial, only one phrase was displayed at a time and remained visible for the duration of the trial. Participants were instructed to proceed at a quick but comfortable pace and to fix errors if noticed and as they occurred using BACKSPACE. There was a brief break between each input method, during which participants were informed and presented with an information screen to move on to the next keyboard.
When finished, participants were asked to complete a questionnaire evaluating the quality of both text input methods and their overall experience. An informal interview was also conducted at the end of the experiment to gather the participants' feedback on their experiences.
Design
This study was a 2 × 5 within-subjects design with the following independent variables and levels:
- Keyboard (standard one-handed, split two-handed)
- Block (1, 2, 3, 4, 5)
The following dependent variables were used:
- Entry speed (wpm)
- Error rate (%)
- Keystroke per character (KSPC)
While entry speed and error rate are self-explanatory, KSPC is the number of keystrokes, on average, to generate each character of text in each language using a given text entry technique (MacKenzie, 2002). As a performance metric, KSPC relates to efficiency since it includes the overhead in keystrokes to correct errors, when errors were noticed and corrected by participants.
In addition, data were collected through a questionnaire administered before and after testing. The questionnaire gathered demographic data, including the user's age, sex, and frequency of VR use (on a scale of 0 to 10). A post-trial questionnaire was used to gauge user preference for keyboard type. Users rated both text input techniques on a scale of 1 to 10, with 10 being the best.
In an effort to balance the testing sequence and eliminate learning effects, participants were divided into two groups. The first group was tested with a standard one-handed keyboard first and then with the split two-handed. The order of testing was reversed for the second group.
Each participant went through five blocks of trials, with each block including five phrases. Testing lasted around 40 minutes for each participant. The total number of trials was 14 participants × 2 keyboards × 5 blocks × 5 phrases = 700.
RESULTS AND DISCUSSION
Some participants accidentally pressed the ENTER key during a trial. This caused the trial to end prematurely and potentially compromised the data. Trials with an incomplete phrase and with >50% error rate were deemed outliers. Eleven such trials (1.6%) were identified and removed from the analyses.The results of the study indicated that the group (i.e., the order in which the text input methods were tested) did not significantly affect entry speed (F1,12 = 2.07, p > .05). This suggests that counterbalancing, which aimed to offset any potential learning effects, was successful.
The data on entry speed, error rate, and KSPC (keystrokes per character) are presented in the following sections and are depicted in the corresponding figures.
Entry Speed
Across all 700 phrases, the grand mean for entry speed was 14.7 wpm. The standard one-handed keyboard had an average entry speed of 14.1 wpm, which was 7.5% slower than the split two-handed keyboard, which had an average entry speed of 15.2 wpm. See Figure 5. The difference was statistically significant (F1,12 = 19.1, p < .001).
Figure 5: Entry speed (wpm) by keyboard. Error bars show ±1 SD.Even though the standard one-handed keyboard was slower, both keyboards demonstrated a similar learning progression over the 25 phrase iterations, which were divided into five blocks. It was noted that the one-handed keyboard showed a positive peak in performance around the third block, while the split two-handed keyboard showed a slight decline in entry speed around the same time. However, both keyboards eventually returned to an upward trend that continued until the end of the last block. Overall, the standard one-handed keyboard saw a 4.8% increase in entry speed from block 1 to block 5, while the split two-handed keyboard experienced an 8.2% increase. Despite these differences in performance, the effect of the block on entry speed was not statistically significant speed (F4,48 = 1.94, p > .05). The learning trend is seen in Figure 6.
Figure 6: Entry speed (wpm) by block.
Error Rate
The study compared the error rates of two keyboards: a standard one-handed keyboard and a split two-handed keyboard. The grand mean for error rate was 1.00%. The standard one-handed keyboard had an error rate of 1.07%, while the split two-handed keyboard had an error rate of 0.94%. Comparing block 1 to block 5, the error rates for the standard one-handed keyboard were relatively consistent with values of 1.18% (block 1) and 1.08% (block 5). The corresponding error rates increased for the split two-handed keyboard, from 0.65% (block 1) to 1.26% (block 5). See in Figure 8. The difference in error rates between the two keyboards was not statistically significant (F1,12 = 0.46, ns).
Figure 7: Error rate (%) by the keyboard. Error bars show ±1 SE.The standard deviation for the standard one-handed keyboard was 2.49, while the standard deviation for the split two-handed keyboard was 2.40, as shown in the error rate bars in Figure 7. This indicates variance in the error rates for the participants using these keyboards, with some participants making significantly more errors than others. It is possible that the higher standard deviation could be attributed to the participants' individual typing abilities, or the difficulty of the tasks. Further research is needed to fully understand the factors contributing to the variability in error rates across participants.
Figure 8: Error rate (%) by block.
Keystrokes Per Character (KSPC)
The KSPC for the standard one-handed keyboard was 1.05 while it was similar at 1.06 for the split-two handed keyboard. See Figure 9. The values reflect a small overhead in keystrokes to correct errors.
Figure 9: KSPC (%) by keyboard. Error bars shown ±1 SD.
Participant Feedback
The questionnaire asked participants to indicate their preferred keyboard and the favourability of each keyboard. The results for preference were evenly split with seven participants indicating a preference for each keyboard. Favourability was given on a scale out of 10 with 10 being awesome. The means were 8.1 for the standard one-hand keyboard and 7.2 for split two-handed keyboard, indicating good overall favourability for both keyboards.
CONCLUSION
In conclusion, the aim of this study was to compare the performance of two virtual keyboards in VR environments and determine which one was more effective and efficient for text input. Results show that the split two-handed keyboard was significantly faster than the standard one-handed keyboard when it came to text entry, with a 7.5% advantage in speed. Both keyboards had similar error rates and keystrokes per character, indicating that the difference in speed was not due to an increase in errors.Questionnaire data revealed that half the participants preferred the standard one-handed keyboard, and half preferred the split two-handed keyboard, indicating that personal preference may also play a role in the choice of keyboard. The mean favourability rating for the standard one-handed keyboard was 8.1, compared to 7.2 for the split two-handed keyboard, indicating that participants generally had positive opinions of both keyboards. In the context of VR typing, the slight difference in favourability between the standard one-handed keyboard and the split two-handed keyboard could be due to the one-handed keyboard only requiring the use of one hand at a time, allowing users to rest their other hand. The feedback from the participants did mention the issue of arm tiredness. Additionally, the two-handed keyboard may require more coordination and hand movements, which could potentially impact typing speed.
These findings suggest that the choice of keyboard may depend on a variety of factors, including the user's personal preferences, typing abilities, and the specific tasks being performed. Further research is needed to fully understand the factors that contribute to the observed differences in performance.
REFERENCES
1. Boletsis, C., & Kongsvik, S. (2019). Controller-based text-input techniques for virtual reality: An empirical comparison. Int J Virtual Reality, 19(10), 1-8. doi:10.20870/IJVR.2019.19.3.2917
2. Bowman, D. A., Rhoton, C. J., & Pinho, M. S. (2002). Text input techniques for immersive virtual environments: An empirical comparison. Proc HFES '02, 2154-2158. Santa Monica, CA, HFES. doi:10.1177/154193120204602611
3. Chen, S., Wang, J., Guerra, S., Mittal, N., & Prakkamakul, S. (2019). Exploring word-gesture text entry techniques in virtual reality. Proc CHI '19, 1-6. New York, ACM. doi:10.1145/3290607.3312762
4. Dube, T.J., Arif, A.S. (2019). Text Entry in Virtual Reality: A Comprehensive Review of the Literature. Proc HCII '19. Berlin, Springer. doi:10.1007/978-3-030-22643-5_33
5. Grubert, J., Witzani, L., Ofek, E., Pahud, M., Kranz, M., & Kristensson, P. O. (2018). Effects of hand representations for typing in virtual reality. Proc Conf on Virtual Reality and 3D User Interfaces - VR '18, 151-158. New York, IEEE. doi:10.1109/VR.2018.8446250
6. Grubert, J., Witzani, L., Ofek, E., Pahud, M., Kranz, M., & Kristensson, P. O. (2018). Text entry in immersive head-mounted display-based virtual reality using standard keyboards. Proc Conf on Virtual Reality and 3D User Interfaces - VR '18, 159-166. New York, IEEE. doi:10.1109/VR.2018.8446059
7. He, Z., Lutteroth, C., & Perlin, K. (2022). TapGazer: Text entry with finger tapping and gaze-directed word selection. Proc CHI '22, 337-342. New York, ACM. doi:10.1145/3491102.3501838
8. MacKenzie, I. S. (2002). KSPC (keystrokes per character) as a characteristic of text entry techniques. Proc Int Conf on Mobile HCI, 195-210. Berlin, Springer. doi:10.1007/3-540-45756-9_16
9. MacKenzie, I. S., & Soukoreff, R. W. (2003). Phrase sets for evaluating text entry techniques. Proc CHI '03, 754-755. New York, ACM. doi:10.1145/765891.765971
10. McGill, M., Boland, D., Murray-Smith, R., & Brewster, S. (2015). A dose of reality: Overcoming usability challenges in VR head-mounted displays. Proc CHI '15, 2143-2152. New York, ACM. doi:10.1145/2702123.2702382
11. Rajanna, V., & Hansen, J. P. (2018). Gaze typing in virtual reality: Impact of keyboard design, selection method, and motion. Proc ETRA '18, 15-20. New York, ACM. doi:10.1145/3204493.3204541
12. Rempel, D. (2008). The split keyboard: An ergonomics success story. Human Factors, 50(3), 385-392. doi:10.1518/001872008X312215
13. Smith, M. L., Pickens, A. W., Ahn, S., Ory, M. G., DeJoy, D. M., Young, K., Bishop, G., & Congleton, J. J. (2015). Typing performance and body discomfort among overweight and obese office workers: A pilot study of keyboard modification. Applied Ergonomics, 46, 30-37. doi:10.1016/j.apergo.2014.06.004
14. Soukoreff, R. W., & MacKenzie, I. S. (2003). Metrics for text entry research: An evaluation of MSD and KSPC, and a new unified error metric. Proc CHI '03, 113-120. New York, ACM. doi:10.1145/642611.642632
15. Speicher, M., Feit, A. M., Ziegler, P., & Kruger, A. (2018). Selection-based text entry in virtual reality. Proc CHI '18, 1-13. ACM. doi:10.1145/3173574.3174221
16. Walker, J., Li, B., Vertanen, K., & Kuhl, S. (2017). Efficient typing on a visually occluded physical keyboard. Proc CHI '17, 5457-5461. New York, ACM. doi:10.1145/3025453.3025783
17. Yu, C., Gu, Y., Yang, Z., Yi, X., Luo, H., & Shi, Y. (2017). Tap, dwell, or gesture? Exploring head-based text entry techniques for HMDs. Proc CHI '17, 4479-4488. New York, ACM. doi:10.1145/3025453.3025964